March 7, 2022

Tiny worms make complex decisions, too

Salk scientists surprised to discover flexible decision-making capabilities in a worm with just 302 neurons

Salk News


Tiny worms make complex decisions, too

Salk scientists surprised to discover flexible decision-making capabilities in a worm with just 302 neurons

LA JOLLA—How does an animal make decisions? Scientists have spent decades trying to answer this question by focusing on the cells and connections of the brain that might be involved. Salk scientists are taking a different approach—analyzing behavior, not neurons. They were surprised to find that worms can take multiple factors into account and choose between two different actions, despite having only 302 neurons compared to approximately 86 billion in humans.

C. elegans worm (right) escaping the predatory P. pacificus worm (left).
C. elegans worm (right) escaping the predatory P. pacificus worm (left).
Click here for a high-resolution image.
Credit: Salk Institute

The findings, published in Current Biology on March 7, 2022, have important implications for the way researchers assess motivation and cognitive abilities in animals. What’s more, the study demonstrates that complex decision-making capabilities could be encoded in small biological and artificial networks.

“Our study shows you can use a simple system such as the worm to study something complex, like goal-directed decision-making. We also demonstrated that behavior can tell us a lot about how the brain works,” says senior author Sreekanth Chalasani, associate professor in Salk’s Molecular Neurobiology Laboratory. “Even simple systems like worms have different strategies, and they can choose between those strategies, deciding which one works well for them in a given situation. That provides a framework for understanding how these decisions are made in more complex systems, such as humans.”

Whether eating prey or defending its food source, the predatory worm Pristionchus pacificus relies on biting. The team’s challenge was to determine the worm’s intentions when it bites.

The researchers found that P. pacificus chooses between two foraging strategies for biting its prey and competitor, another worm called Caenorhabditis elegans: 1) predatory strategy, in which its goal for biting is to kill prey, or 2) territorial strategy, in which biting is instead used to force C. elegans away from a food source. P. pacificus chooses the predatory strategy against larval C. elegans, which is easy to kill. In contrast, P. pacificus selects the territorial strategy against adult C. elegans, which is difficult to kill and outcompetes P. pacificus for food.

From left: Sreekanth Chalasani and Kathleen Quach
From left: Sreekanth Chalasani and Kathleen Quach
Click here for a high-resolution image.
Credit: Salk Institute

To the team, it appeared that P. pacificus weighed the costs and benefits of multiple potential outcomes of an action—behavior that’s familiar in vertebrates but unexpected in a worm.

“Scientists have always assumed that worms were simple—when P. pacificus bites we thought that was always for a singular predatory purpose,” says first author Kathleen Quach, a postdoctoral fellow in Chalasani’s lab. “Actually, P. pacificus is versatile and can use the same action, biting C. elegans, to achieve different long-term goals. I was surprised to find that P. pacificus could leverage what seemed like failed predation into successful and goal-directed territoriality.”

In the future, the scientists would like to determine which of P. pacificus’ cost-benefit calculations are hard-wired or flexible. They hope more research like this will help further uncover the molecular underpinnings of decision-making.

The research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (5R01MH113905), the W.M. Keck Foundation, the National Science Foundation, Salk Women & Science and a Paul F. Glenn Foundation Postdoctoral Fellowship.

DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.033

PUBLICATION INFORMATION

JOURNAL

Current Biology

TITLE

Flexible reprogramming of Pristionchus pacificus motivation for attacking Caenorhabditis elegans in predator-prey competition

AUTHORS

Kathleen T. Quach & Sreekanth H. Chalasani

Research Areas

For More Information

Office of Communications
Tel: (858) 453-4100
press@salk.edu

The Salk Institute For Biological Studies:

Unlocking the secrets of life itself is the driving force behind the Salk Institute. Our team of world-class, award-winning scientists pushes the boundaries of knowledge in areas such as neuroscience, cancer research, aging, immunobiology, plant biology, computational biology and more. Founded by Jonas Salk, developer of the first safe and effective polio vaccine, the Institute is an independent, nonprofit research organization and architectural landmark: small by choice, intimate by nature, and fearless in the face of any challenge.